9781119801702
ISBN :Возрастное ограничение : 0
Дата обновления : 25.12.2020
233В 230
234В 231
235В 232
236В 233
237В 234
238В 235
239В 236
240В 237
241В 238
242В 239
243В 240
244В 241
245В 243
246В 244
247В 245
248В 246
249В 247
250В 248
Introduction to Experimental Linguistics
Christelle Gillioz
Sandrine Zufferey
First published 2020 in Great Britain and the United States by ISTE Ltd and John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Apart from any fair dealing for the purposes of research or private study, or criticism or review, as permitted under the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988, this publication may only be reproduced, stored or transmitted, in any form or by any means, with the prior permission in writing of the publishers, or in the case of reprographic reproduction in accordance with the terms and licenses issued by the CLA. Enquiries concerning reproduction outside these terms should be sent to the publishers at the undermentioned address:
ISTE Ltd
27-37 St George’s Road
London SW19 4EU
UK
www.iste.co.uk (http://www.iste.co.uk)
John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
111 River Street
Hoboken, NJ 07030
USA
www.wiley.com (http://www.wiley.com)
В© ISTE Ltd 2020
The rights of Christelle Gillioz and Sandrine Zufferey to be identified as the authors of this work have been asserted by them in accordance with the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988.
Library of Congress Control Number: 2020943938
British Library Cataloguing-in-Publication Data
A CIP record for this book is available from the British Library
ISBN 978-1-78630-418-6
Preface
This book aims to present the theoretical and methodological principles of experimental linguistics in an accessible manner. It intends to offer an overall vision of the field, so as to help the non-initiated audience to become familiar with the necessary concepts for carrying out linguistic experiments. The elements discussed in this book can particularly serve as a basis for a critical understanding of the results published in the scientific literature and as a starting point for carrying out experiments.
Since the field of experimental linguistics is rich and varied, both in terms of the phenomena studied and of the methods employed, it is impossible to offer an exhaustive presentation. The choice of aspects introduced in this book aims to provide an overview of the different possibilities available to those wishing to carry out an experimental study about language. For every aspect developed in the chapters of the book, there exist specific works which, due to their complexity and the prerequisites they demand, are often reserved for an expert audience. This is why we have deliberately chosen to select the information we deem essential for building a knowledge base that will later enable readers to explore the scientific literature and other works on this topic. Therefore, the emphasis will be placed on understanding the scientific approach and the methodological principles underlying the construction of experiments, and on analyzing the data which results from these experiments. In regards to research methods, we chose to make a presentation of the most accessible methods for linguists. In order to illustrate the many possibilities for applying such methods, we have provided examples drawn from different fields in linguistics. Finally, a list of more specific resources and available tools is provided at the end of each chapter, in order to encourage the interested reader to deepen and put into practice the knowledge acquired in this book.
This book begins with an introductory chapter, offering a general overview of the principles underlying experimental methodology, as well as the key concepts which will be developed in the rest of the chapters.
Chapter 2 (#u04c2cebf-2633-50e5-824e-a9e10e997cd2) goes through the various points the researcher should comply with in order to conduct a valid and reliable experiment, thus making it possible to infer solid conclusions. First, we will define the concepts of validity and reliability and then discuss the notion of variables, as well as present different options for measuring such variables. We will pay special attention to the stages involved in the transformation of the research question into an experimentally testable hypothesis.
Chapters 3 (#u757dc413-f351-5b1a-8315-0d05a70e5cca)–5 (#uee748d60-ee5c-5dda-a289-b01987d6379c) are dedicated to the different methods used for studying language production (Chapter 3 (#u757dc413-f351-5b1a-8315-0d05a70e5cca)) and language comprehension, focusing not only on the results of the comprehension process (Chapter 4 (#u3e247f59-c006-53a9-80b8-cc5d1e063a40)), but also on the process itself (Chapter 5 (#uee748d60-ee5c-5dda-a289-b01987d6379c)).
Chapter 6 (#uc8df27c8-0777-528b-815a-dd0c80557d4b) presents the main practical aspects associated with the construction of an experiment, such as the various possibilities offered by different types of experimental designs, the criteria for choosing the experimental material, the stages involved in an experiment, the aspects related to data collection, as well as the ethical principles that should be observed while carrying out research with human participants.
Finally, Chapter 7 (#u45fdd36d-d225-53e3-9134-bba5a7e08a6a) offers an introduction to the analysis of quantitative data, aiming to summarize the key elements for understanding descriptive and inferential statistics, as found in the scientific literature devoted to experimental linguistics. This chapter will also emphasize the peculiarities of the data acquired through linguistic experiments, namely the interdependence of observations. Then, we will introduce mixed linear models that can be used to analyze such types of data.
Christelle GILLIOZ
Sandrine ZUFFEREY
August 2020
1
Experimental Linguistics: General Principles
We start this chapter by outlining the foundations of the experimental methodology and its main features. Then, we discuss the advantages and disadvantages of this type of methodology, as well as the main arguments in favor of its use in the field of linguistics. Last, we present a series of resources offering access to research in experimental linguistics.
1.1. The scientific process
The experimental methodology in linguistics is part of a scientific approach for studying language. It aims to observe language facts from an objective and quantitative point of view. The general idea behind this approach is that it is impossible to rely on one’s own intuitions in order to understand the world. Quite the contrary, it is necessary to observe objective data reflecting reality. For example, by simply observing the world around us, and relying solely on our own intuition, we might believe that the Earth is flat. This is why the scientific approach, used in fields such as psychology or physics, is based on specific principles and stages, instead of relying on the intuition of scientists. Let us briefly go through these stages:
The first stage in the scientific process involves the observation of concrete phenomena and the subsequent generalization of observations, in order to build a scientific fact: a fact which does not depend on a specific place, time, object or person. At this first stage, it is also possible to trace certain regularities concerning the emergence of a phenomenon, and to try to define the conditions in which such phenomenon generally appears. So, let us illustrate this process by reviewing the stages involved in the discovery of gravitation. This finding is usually attributed to Isaac Newton, who is said to have had a revelation after seeing several apples fall from a tree. As he watched the apples fall, Newton wondered why the apples always fell in a perpendicular direction from the apple tree to the ground, never to the side or upwards.
During the second stage, all of the scientific facts concerning the same phenomenon may prompt the development of a law or theory aimed at explaining such facts. A theory synthesizes knowledge about a phenomenon at a given moment and is therefore provisional, insofar as it can evolve according to new knowledge. We should make it clear that the notion of theory in science is rather distant from the meaning of the word theory as we use it in everyday language. While this word can be used to refer to personal ideas or reasoning mechanisms, its use in the scientific field only applies to coherent and well-established principles or explanations. Going back to our example, in Newton’s time, two models coexisted for describing the movement of bodies: one followed Galileo’s law and was devoted to terrestrial bodies, whereas the other was oriented by Kepler’s law and made reference to celestial bodies. On the basis of this knowledge and his own observations, Newton suggested the existence of a force which made objects attract one another and which could explain the movements of both celestial and terrestrial bodies.
At the third stage, a theory is capable of predicting the emergence of observable facts, or to put it differently, to formulate precise hypotheses which can be put to the test. In order to test these hypotheses, it is necessary to collect a large amount of data and check whether they support the initial theory. In this way, it is possible to know to what extent we can rely on our theory. The more the predictions made on the basis of the theory are fulfilled, that is, the more the data collected corresponds to what might be expected according to the theory, the higher the confidence level will be. Otherwise, if the predictions did not come true, the theory should be put into question and re-examined. Newton’s law of universal gravitation has made it possible to predict and explain the movement of the tides thanks to the moon’s gravitational pull on the Earth, the elliptical movement of celestial bodies or the equatorial bulge.
In summary, the scientific approach is a circular and dynamic process, originating in the reality of the facts, abstracting itself from them in an attempt to explain them, and then approaching them again to check the validity of the explanation.
1.1.1. Qualitative and quantitative approaches
It is possible to investigate a research question in different ways and from different perspectives. Let us imagine that you wish to study second language acquisition within the context of linguistic immersion. The first way of doing this could be to contact students attending your university for a language stay and to interview them. These interviews can later be viewed to analyze the opinions of students regarding their experience during their stay, their feelings on its advantages and disadvantages, or their opinion on the impact of such a stay on their linguistic competences. By doing this, you would be carrying out what is called qualitative research.
The qualitative approach helps us to explore and understand a phenomenon by studying it in detail and trying to take hold of it in a holistic manner, based on the meanings that people assign to the phenomenon. This type of research takes a long time when conducting interviews and interpreting the results; hence, only a small number of individuals can be questioned. Due to this characteristic, the results of a qualitative study are strongly anchored to the context in which the study was carried out, and cannot be generalized to other people or to other contexts. This is not a problem insofar, as qualitative studies do not aim to make such a generalization. The subjectivity of the individuals involved in the study is acknowledged as an integral part of qualitative research. This methodology is built on the principles of a constructivist vision of knowledge, according to which there is not only one, but many realities construed by people’s interpretations and the meanings they attribute to events or things, on the basis of their own experience.
When reading this first proposal for investigating second language acquisition within a context of linguistic immersion, you might think that although it may be interesting to know learners’ opinions about their experience in a language stay, you also desire to know more about the benefits of such a stay on the evolution of their linguistic competences. The conclusions drawn based on the opinions of a few interviewees may not reflect the reality of all learners. It is possible that the interviewees could subjectively overestimate or underestimate the evolution of their skills, or that these particular cases do not mirror the typical experience learners have during a language stay. One possibility, to obtain more objective data on the advantages of a language stay for improving linguistic competences, could be to take into account the experience of more people and to measure their linguistic competences at the start and end of the stay, for example, with an assessment test. By comparing the results before and after the stay with the help of a statistical test, you could determine whether the students’ linguistic skills have evolved and in what aspect. If you chose this second option, your research would follow a quantitative methodology, in the sense that your conclusions would be drawn from the analysis of numerical data pertaining to a large number of people, and objectively assessed through a test. Your results would depend little on the respondents, their subjective perceptions or your interpretation of their declarations. If learners have really benefited from their language stay, this should be reflected in their results to the test, probably higher at the end than at the beginning of the stay, and this is what you would measure directly.
This example illustrates to what extent quantitative research differs from qualitative research, in that it aims to observe quantifiable elements and to measure a phenomenon. The techniques used for measuring a phenomenon can be extremely varied, depending on how the phenomenon is defined. Going back to our previous example, it is possible to measure language proficiency using a general language test (such as the placement tests used in language schools). Another way of doing this would be to count the number of mistakes students make in a grammar test or to measure the size of their second language lexicon. Choosing the proper measures for undertaking research is a big question in itself. We will return to this in Chapter 2 (#u04c2cebf-2633-50e5-824e-a9e10e997cd2), where we will discuss the different stages of choosing the measures involved in an experiment.
Quantitative research also differs from qualitative research in terms of the type of reasoning on which it is based. We have seen that in qualitative research, we draw upon data in order to outline a structure. In this case, data works as a source of interpretations and explanations upon which hypotheses will be formulated. This type of reasoning, starting from data and leading towards a theory, is called inductive reasoning. On the contrary, quantitative research follows deductive reasoning: it draws on theory in order to formulate hypotheses which will later be verified by data acquired in the field. When choosing a deductive approach, it is necessary to build a preliminary hypothesis, on which the research will be based and that will guide the researchers’ methodological choices.
Going back to the example of learners within an immersion context, there are a large number of hypotheses that could be formulated by using the link between language stay and language proficiency. The first hypothesis could be that a language stay improves second language skills. A second hypothesis, similar to the first, but involving a different research methodology, could be that people who have spent time on a language stay have acquired better skills than those who have not. In order to verify the second hypothesis, we would have to test two groups of learners who may or may not have benefited from linguistic immersion, instead of one group of students before and after the stay. A third hypothesis could focus on one specific aspect of language proficiency, such as pronunciation in a foreign language (accent). We might imagine that the learners who have spent some time on a language stay may have a better pronunciation (an accent closer to that of the native speakers), than those who have not. In order to test this third hypothesis, two groups of students would be required, but this time they would be assessed on their pronunciation.
Even if they differ in their formulation and in the type of elements they have put to the test, the hypotheses mentioned above share a common feature, which is that they all postulate a relationship between what we call variables. In all the hypotheses, the first variable corresponds to linguistic immersion. In the first and second hypotheses, the second variable is the proficiency level in the second language. In the third hypothesis, the second variable corresponds to a weaker non-native accent. We will discuss the notion of variables in further detail in Chapter 2 (#u04c2cebf-2633-50e5-824e-a9e10e997cd2). For the time being, it is important to understand that a variable is something that varies, and can take different values. For example, a variable can be the age of participants in a study, which would result in a broad number of values. A second variable could be the fact of wearing glasses, or eye color, etc. These variables adopt fewer values: either yes or no for wearing glasses, and blue, brown, green or other for eye color.
Let us now take the example of a variable studied in language science: bilingualism. At first glance, this variable may seem to only adopt two values: either bilingual or monolingual. However, things get more complicated when we have to define what we mean by bilingual. For example, we may decide that anyone having knowledge of a second language is bilingual. In that case, there would be great heterogeneity within the bilingual group, containing people who can only speak or understand a second language superficially, and people capable of perfectly mastering both languages. A corollary of such a definition would be that very few people would belong to the monolingual group, since many people are familiar with one or more languages, apart from their mother tongue. On the other extreme, we could consider belonging to the bilingual group as only those with a perfect command of their second language. In this case, the bilingual group as would be more homogeneous, in the sense that all those belonging to it would have similar competences in their second language. But this definition raises additional questions: what do we mean by perfect command and how can command be measured? This example illustrates the need to clearly and precisely define the variables investigated in a research process. This definition procedure is called the operationalization of a research question. It represents a crucial phase in quantitative research, and we will discuss it in depth in Chapter 2 (#u04c2cebf-2633-50e5-824e-a9e10e997cd2).
To summarize, quantitative research aims to investigate the relationship between two or more variables. To do this, it starts from a hypothesis and defines the measures used for studying the chosen variables. Then, it relies on digital data collected from a large number of people and analyzes such data using statistical tests, in order to generalize the results.
1.1.2. Observational research and experimental research
Quantitative approaches in linguistics make an important difference between observational research and experimental research. The first example of a research tool, the questionnaire, is frequently used in linguistics to collect data in a quantitative manner. A questionnaire is a set of questions aimed at collecting different types of information about speakers, such as personal characteristics, their use of certain words or linguistic structures, or their point of view about certain linguistic phenomena. Let us now imagine that you wish to know whether there is a difference in the way that French speakers from France, Belgium and Switzerland refer to a yogurt. As Avanzi (2019) did, you could directly ask a large number of French, Belgian and Swiss people to tell you which of the two possible names, yaourt or yoghourt, they use on a daily basis. By counting the responses of more than 7,000 people, Avanzi showed that the form yaourt is mainly used in France, whereas it is never used in Switzerland, where yoghourt is the only form in use. In Belgium, the choice of yaourt and yoghourt varies from region to region.
In a slightly different way, instead of relying on the answers of people in a questionnaire, you could use linguistic data retrieved from natural productions and carry out a corpus study. In such studies, linguistic productions in the form of texts, audio or video recordings are used with the aim of counting the number of word occurrences, a grammatical form or any linguistic characteristic. In order to research the uses of yaourt or yoghourt in France, Belgium and Switzerland, first it would be necessary to select corpora comprising linguistic productions collected from these different regions. This data could come from French, Belgian and Swiss newspapers, for example. The number of occurrences of each form could be counted in each corpus and then compared, in order to reveal differences in the use of these forms from country to country.
Another way of studying quantitative data is to examine the link between two variables. Let us imagine that you wish to study the relation between learners’ age and their ability to acquire a second language. Extensive research has already been devoted to this topic and suggests that the older people are when learning a second language, the more difficult it is for them to reach a high level of proficiency (see DeKeyser and Larson-Hall (2005) for a review). In order to confirm (or refute) this hypothesis, you could test a large number of people who start learning a language at different ages and measure their language proficiency after a certain period of time. In this example, the first variable, the age when learning begins, is a quantitative variable. Likewise, the second variable, language proficiency, can be measured quantitatively using a language test. Using an appropriate statistical test, it is possible to show the existence of a link between these two variables. This type of procedure is called correlational research and unveils the degree of dependence between two variables, which is called correlation. In the case of our example, if age plays a role in second language acquisition, the correlation obtained by our test would show that the older a person is when the process of learning a language begins, the lower their mastery of the language will be after a certain learning period.
The various studies described above correspond to research based on data observation. This type of research is generally used when, for practical or ethical reasons, it is necessary to observe variables from the outside. In this type of research, researchers do not interfere with the object of study, but observe the relationship between two variables at a given moment. As a consequence, the results of an observational study must be kept at a descriptive level, since it is not possible to infer a causal relation between two variables. In our example of a correlational study, the age when learning begins is related to language proficiency, but it is not possible to state that an increase in age is the cause for the decrease in language proficiency. It might be possible that other variables not considered in our research can also explain the relationship between the variables examined. We could imagine, for example, that the context in which second language acquisition takes place is not the same depending on the age when the learning process begins. It is likely that when young children learn a second language, this takes place within a family setting, where parents may speak different languages or a different language from that of the external environment. When older people start learning a language, it is probable that they grew up in a monolingual linguistic environment and later discovered a second language at school, or when moving to another country, for example. The type of linguistic exchanges may also differ depending on age, as well as the motivation to learn, cognitive skills or many other variables. These external variables that are left aside during research are called confounding variables and are related to the two variables examined, age and language proficiency. It could be, that language learning conditions rather than age itself can account for the differences in language levels. Since it is impossible to distinguish the variables examined, from confounding variables, research based on the observation of data should not draw a conclusion from a causal relation between two variables.
In order to determine a causal relation between two variables, it is necessary to exclude any confounding variable. By using experimental methodology, the variables of interest can be manipulated to determine what effect a variable has on another variable, regardless of other possibly interfering variables. In other words, rather than observing natural data, the experimental methodology defines the conditions under which a phenomenon could be observed and then sets up an experiment in which these conditions can be manipulated, in order to measure their influence on the phenomenon under investigation. In the rest of this chapter, we will describe in more detail the various characteristics of experimental research.
Все книги на сайте предоставены для ознакомления и защищены авторским правом